“Sniff Out”
The ability of dogs to “sniff out” certain diseases including COVID-19 infection and cancer has been well documented over recent years. Now, a new study reveals dogs can detect a person’s stress level by odor alone.
Dogs have a long history alongside humans, giving them an amazing ability to read human cues. Dogs also possess an incredible sense of smell, which enables them to detect diseases, such as COVID and lung cancer, in humans from odor alone.
Whether dogs’ capabilities extend to detecting odors associated with psychological states has been explored far less.
When people are stressed, there are hormonal and nervous system changes that alter the kinds of odors produced by the body. My colleagues and I wanted to know if dogs can discriminate between odor samples taken from the same person before and after becoming stressed. To do this, we took ideas from the field of biomedical detection dogs (sniffer dogs in a laboratory setting) and combined these ideas with techniques used to test dogs’ perceptions of odors.
Our results are published in the journal PLOS One.
Psychological Stress
To test whether dogs could detect an odor associated with psychological stress, we attached sensors to the study participants to continuously measure their heart rate and blood pressure. Participants also rated how stressed they were feeling before and after taking part in the task.
Before the task started, participants wiped gauze on the back of their necks, placed it into a sterile glass vial, and exhaled it into the vial. We then got the participants to perform a rapid mental arithmetic task to induce stress in them.
After the task, participants provided another rating of their stress and two additional sweat/breath samples.
The total time between the collection of the relaxed (pre-task) and stressed (post-task) samples was four minutes, reducing the likelihood that the participants experienced changes other than the onset of stress.
We only included samples in the study if the person reported finding the task stressful, and both their heart rate and blood pressure had increased during the lesson. We presented samples from 36 people to the dogs.
The Training Process
The dogs included in this study were pets, volunteered by their owners, who were trained using positive reinforcement by researchers in a laboratory once a week. Before formal data collection began, dogs were taught to communicate that they were picking a sample by standing and freezing above it for several seconds or sitting in front of it – we called this their “alert behavior”.
The dogs were then taught a matching game, where they learned to discriminate between samples with known odor differences. Once it was established that the dogs were successful at this, they were ready to be tested.
At testing, we tasked the dogs with discriminating between a person’s samples taken before and after the arithmetic task. To teach the dogs what odor they should be looking for in each testing session, they were first shown the person’s stress sweat/breath sample alongside two “control samples” – clean gauze in glass vials with no sweat or breath.
The dogs were allowed to sniff all three samples and were rewarded for alerting the researchers to the sweat/breath sample.
After ten exposures, a second breath/sweat sample was added to the line-up: the same person’s relaxed sample. Here began the test of discrimination, which took place over the next 20 trials. It was the dogs’ job to communicate, through their alert behavior, which sample they perceived as the same as the one shown in the previous ten trials, that is, which sample smelled like the stressed sample. Because dogs might use other information to help them make a choice, we included both visual and odor controls.
Smelling Different
If these two odors smell the same to the dog, we would expect them to pick either by chance. If the two odors smell different, they would be able to consistently find the odor first presented to them: the stress odor. Each sample set from participants was used only once, so the dogs saw samples from a different person during each session.
From the very first time the dogs were exposed to these samples, they perceived the samples as smelling different. The dogs correctly chose the stressed sample in 94% of 720 trials, showing that the participants’ psychological experience of the arithmetic task had resulted in their bodies emitting odors in their breath and sweat that the dogs could detect.
It should be noted that this study does not determine whether the dogs perceived the stress samples as reflecting a negative emotional state. In a real-life setting, dogs likely use various contextual cues, such as our body language, tone of voice, or breathing rate, to help them understand a situation. However, the results provide firm evidence that odor is also a component that dogs can pick up on.
Establishing that dogs can detect an odor associated with human stress provides deeper knowledge of the human-dog relationship and adds to our understanding of how dogs perceive and interact with human psychological states. This knowledge could also be useful for the training of anxiety and PTSD service dogs that are currently trained to respond predominantly to visual cues.
Original Research: Open access.
“Dogs can discriminate between human baseline and psychological stress condition odors” by Clara Wilson et al. PLOS ONE